Call for Commentators for PSYCHE article

x91007 at pitvax.xx.rmit.edu.au x91007 at pitvax.xx.rmit.edu.au
Thu Jul 14 09:15:31 EST 1994


CALL FOR COMMENTARIES

PSYCHE, an electronic journal devoted to the interdisciplinary study of
consciousness, will shortly publish an article by Bernard Baars entitled
"A thoroughly empirical approach to consciousness". 

We would like to invite you to give your view on this article.

If you are interested in writing a commentary on the article please contact 
us first giving a brief outline of your proposed commentary to see whether it 
will fit within the focus of PSYCHE. If you have any questions please do not 
hesitate to contact me. 

If you would like to subscribe to PSYCHE send the following message to 
<listserv at nki.bitnet>:

SUBSCRIBE PSYCHE-L YourFirstName YourLastName

There is a related discussion list - Psyche-D - to subscribe to this send the 
following message to <listserv at nki.bitnet>:

SUBSCRIBE PSYCHE-D YourFirstName YourLastName


Yours sincerely,

Winand Dittrich
w.h.dittrich at herts.ac.uk

-----------------------------------------------------
A THOROUGHLY EMPIRICAL APPROACH TO CONSCIOUSNESS

Bernard J. Baars

ABSTRACT:  When are psychologists entitled to call a certain theoretical
construct "consciousness?"  Over the past few decades cognitive
psychologists have reintroduced almost the entire conceptual vocabulary
of common sense psychology, but now in a way that is tied explicitly to
reliable empirical observations, and to compelling and increasingly
adequate theoretical models.  Nevertheless, until the past few years
most cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists avoided dealing with
consciousness. Today there is an increasing willingness to do so. 

But is "consciousness" different from other theoretical entities like
"working memory" or "mental imagery"?  Some argue that under no
circumstances can empirical science speak of consciousness AS SUCH,
while others claim that the scientific goal is "knowing what it is like
to be a bat" --- to share an organism's conscious experience  (Nagel,
1974). The "bat" argument is ominously reminiscent of the protracted
debate on the consciousness of ants and amoebas that caused so much
uneasiness in psychology around 1900. It seems to  demand that we first
solve the mind-body problem AS A CONDITION of doing sensible science,
and thereby creates the risk of endless, fruitless controversy. The
endless philosophical debate about consciousness helped trigger the
behavioristic revolution about 1913, which threw out the baby of
consciousness with the bathwater of perennial, circular debate. WeUve
been that way; let's not go back to it. 

This paper maintains that the position of behavioristic denial is far
too restrictive, but that the Bat Criterion is far too demanding ---
that in fact, we only need to specify comparable pairs of psychological
phenomena that differ only in the fact that one member of any pair is
conscious, while the other is not.  This "method of contrastive
analysis" is a generalization of the experimental method, with
consciousness as a  variable whose interaction with other psychological
and biological phenomena can be assessed in standard ways. As usual in
science, this strategy is pragmatic: If it appears to yield sensible
results, it can be a stepping-stone toward further understanding. (Crick
and Koch, 1992) 

This  paper describes five sets of well-established pairs of phenomena
that meet these criteria. Others are worked out elsewhere (Baars, 1983,
1988, 1994).  Any adequate theory of conscious experience must satisfy
these demanding but achievable empirical constraints. 

-----------------------------------------------------------------------
GUIDELINES FOR COMMENTARIES AND RESPONSES

All contributions are refereed. In the event that an article needs to
be modified for publication the author will be responsible for making
any alterations requested by the editors.  When submitting articles
please follow the notes below:
 
1. Unless stated otherwise articles should be submitted in the standard
APA (American Psychological Association) Format.  In addition certain
modifications have had to be made to make allowance for the electronic
medium of the journal.
 
2. The maximum line width should not exceed 72 characters, so that
people with any kind of terminal can read it without irritating
line-wrapping. Typically, a commentary/response would have about
800-1000 words, i.e.  100 lines but should not exceed 200 lines. 
 
3. Remember that no matter what word-processing program you use, the
article must be saved in the 'ASCII' or 'Text' or 'DOS Text' format
prior to uploading and sending it as an e-mail message.  Please do not
use any 'funny' characters, namely those with ASCII codes outside the
range 32 to 126 inclusive, as these may not be displayed in the same way
for other people.
 
4. Since attributes such as underlining and italics are not available in
ASCII format please use the following conventions:
 
#bold#
*italics*
~superscript~
%subscript%
_underline_
 
In the printed version these will be turned into the right markups.
 
5. Since page length loses its meaning in the electronic medium, authors
are asked to number sections, and paragraphs within sections.  Thus 1.13
|Cwould refer to Section 1, Paragraph 13 with an article.  Please follow
this convention even if there is only one section within the body of the
work.
 
6. Similarly, the footnoting/endnoting facility in word-processing
programs is not automatically convertible into ASCII/Text/DOS Text.  So
you will have to do a bit of 'manual' conversion.  The footnotes in the
text of the article should be set in 'pointy brackets', e.g. <1>, and
the actual citations should be placed as endnotes at the end of the
article.
 
7. All sections of the text should be divided by blank lines, namely
section headings, paragraphs, entries in the bibliography and individual
footnotes.
 
8. Submissions should be preceded by a header containing the title of
the paper, the name(s) of the author(s), any affiliations, mail and
e-mail addresses, and telephone numbers.  The header should be formatted
like the following example.
 
TITLE: Title of the Paper
AUTHOR: YourFirstName YourLastName
AFFILIATION: YourCollegeorUniversity, YourDepartment
MAIL ADDRESS: StreetAddress, City, State, Zip, Country
E-MAIL ADDRESS: YourEMailAddress
PHONE: Office/(nnn) nnn-nnnn;  Home/(nnn) nnn-nnn
 
9.  After this header there should be an abstract of about 80
words. (Note: An abstract of 150 words at the mandated maximum line
width of 72 characters will be approximately 13 lines long.) 
 
10. PSYCHE articles are intended to be widely distributed.  The plain
text format required to achieve this is obviously restrictive.  As far
as possible, articles should be written without the use of tables,
mathematical formulae, or diagrams.  Where these are necessary, the
author should include where possible ASCII versions in the Plain ASCII
file.
 
It is possible to include more complex figures in the Rich-Text versions
of the journal.  If these are needed for an article they should be
submitted as a second file in either LaTeX, TeX, PostScript, or dvi
formats.  The text of the article should still be submitted as a Plain
ASCII file, with clear indications in the text where figures should be
inserted.
 
12. Authors of accepted material assign to PSYCHE the right to publish
the text both electronically and as printed matter and to make it
available in an electronic archive.  However authors retain copyright of
their work and may republish it in any form they wish so long as PSYCHE
is clearly acknowledged as the original source of publication.

13. As PSYCHE is aimed at an interdisciplinary audience authors are
strongly encouraged to avoid jargon where ever possible, and where technical
terms are unavoidable to carefully introduce any used.
 



More information about the Neur-sci mailing list